Lomonosov Psychology Journal
ISSN 0137-0936
eISSN 2309-9852
En Ru
ISSN 0137-0936
eISSN 2309-9852

Self-esteem and types of creativity in adolescents: relationship and predictors

We present the results of an empirical study of the relationship between self-assessment of creative abilities, originality of creativity (verbal and social) and values. The relevance of the research is related to the demand for creativity as an important competence in the labor market. The ambiguity of the relationship between different types of creativity and self-assessment of creativity, as well as the discrepancy between the results of diagnostics of creative potential using various methods, actualizes the problem of realizing creative potential.

The aim of the work was to study the relationship between different types of creativity, self-assessment of creativity and values, as well as to identify the ratio of creativity indicators diagnosed using the Real Word Divergent Task (RWDT) method and tasks for verbal creativity.

The study involved adolescents aged 15–16 years (N = 50; 26 – men) – students of one of Moscow school. The following methods were used: “Self-assessment of creative abilities” (E. Tunik), tasks on verbal creativity and creativity in social interaction of positive and negative connotations of RWDT, Portrait value questionnaire of Sh. Schwartz.

According to the results, originality is higher in tasks for verbal creativity compared to situations in social interaction; originality in situations of lies and prosocial situations in social interaction significantly correlate; there is no correlation between self-esteem of imagination and creativity indicators measured using the RWDT method and tasks for verbal creativity; self-esteem of creative abilities has a positive relationship with the highest value of “Openness to change”; the predictor of self-esteem of imagination is the value of “Independence of thought”. The results confirmed the hypotheses about specificity of creativity, about the absence of a link between creativity measured as self-report and objective methods; about the presence of a direct link between the highest values of “Openness to change” and the components of creativity measured by self-report, and did not confirm assumption about the absence of a direct link between originality in prosocial situations and situations with negative connotations.

Conclusions are made that 1) creativity is specific in various areas, 2) creativity in situations of lies and prosocial situations in social interaction is a single construct; 3) self-esteem of imagination is not associated with divergent thinking, measured using the RWDT method and tasks for verbal creativity; 4) high self-esteem of imagination is mediated by the motivational goal of thinking creatively, having original ideas, learning new things and phenomena.

References

  1. Alekseeva O.S., Rzhanova I.E. (2019). Relationships between values and personality traits. [Electronic resource]. Psikhologicheskie issledovaniya (Psychological Studies) 12 (63), 10. URL: http://psystudy.ru (date of retrieval: 26.9.2019). (In Russ.)

  2. Antropova M.Yu., Meshkova N.V. (2017). On the Situational Characteristics of Creativity in the Social Interaction of High School StudentsPsikhologo-pedagogicheskie issledovaniya (Psychological-Educational Studies), 9, 3, 175–185. doi:10.17759/psyedu.2017090318. (In Russ.)

  3. Bochkova M.N., Meshkova N.V. (2019). Behavioral Features of Negative and Malevolent Creativity in Adolescents. Psikhologo-pedagogicheskie issledovaniya (Psychological-Educational Studies), 11, 1, 93–106. doi:10.17759/psyedu.2019110108. (In Russ.)

  4. Il'in E.P. (2012). Psychology of creativity, art and giftedness. Saint Petersburg: Piter. (In Russ.)

  5. Meshkova N.V., Shapoval, V.A., Gerasimenko, E.A., Potarykina, M., Meshkov, I.A. (2018a). Personal features and malevolent creativity on the example of cadets and policemen. Psikhologiya i pravo (Psychology and Law), 8, 83–96. doi:10.17759/psylaw.2018080306. (In Russ.)

  6. Meshkova N.V., Enikolopov S.N. (2018) Creativity and Deviance: Communication and Interaction. Psikhologiya. Zhurnal Vysshei shkoly ekonomiki (Psychology. Journal of High School of Economy), 15, 2, 279–290. (In Russ.)

  7. Meshkova N.V. (2018). Interrelation of Malevolent Creativity and Values in Adolescents with Different Levels of Aggression. Psikhologo-pedagogicheskie issledovaniya (Psychological-Educational Studies), 10, 2, 77–87. doi:10.17759/psyedu.2018100207. (In Russ.)

  8. Meshkova N.V., Enikolopov S.N., Mitina O.V., Meshkov I.A.  (2018b). Adaptation of the Malevolent Creativity Behavior Scale. Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie (Psychological Science and Education), 23, 6, 25–40. doi:10.17759/pse.2018230603. (In Russ.)

  9. Pavlova E.M., Kornilova T.V. (2019). The Role of the Triad of Traits “Tolerance for Uncertainty – Emotional Intelligence – Intuition” in Self-Assessed Creativity in Creative Professionals. Psikhologo-pedagogicheskie issledovaniya (Psychological-Educational Studies), 11, 1, 107–117. doi:10.17759/psyedu.2019110109. (In Russ.)

  10. Shvarts Sh. [et al.] (2012). The refined theory of basic individual values: application in Russia. Psikhologiya. Zhurnal Vysshei shkoly ekonomiki (Psychology. Journal of high school of economy), 9, 1, 43–70. (In Russ.)

  11. Arieli S., Tenne-GazitO.(2017) “Values and behavior in a work environment: taking a multi-level perspective” in Values and behavior. In S. Roccas, and L. Sagiv (Eds). Taking a cross-cultural perspective. (pp. 115–141). Berlin: Springer, 115–141.

  12. CTS. Creativity Testing Services [Electronic Resource]. M. Runco (Eds.). ScienceSites: Websites for Scientists, 2019. URL: https://www.creativitytestingservices.com/products/ (date of retrieval: 26.9.2019).

  13. Furnham A., Zhang J., Chamorro-Premuzic T. (2005–2006) The relationship between psychometric and self-estimated intelligence, creativity, personality, and academic achievement. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 25, 2, 119–145.

  14. Gino F., Ariely D. (2012) The dark side of creativity: Original thinkers can be more dishonest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102 (3), 445–459. doi:10.1037/a0026406.

  15. Kapoor H., Khan A. (2019). Creativity in context: Presses and task effects in negative creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 13 (3), 314–321. doi:10.1037/aca0000183

  16. Okuda S. (1991) Creativity and the finding and solving of real-world problems Journal of educational assessment, 9, 45–53. 

  17. Purc E., Laguna M. (2019) Personal Values and Innovative Behavior of Employees. Front. Psychology, 10, 865. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00865.

  18. Reiter-Palmon R. (2012). Evaluation of SelfPerceptions of Creativity: Is It a Useful Criterion. Creativity Research Journal, 24 (2–3), 107–114.

  19. Reiter-Palmon R. (2018) Are the outcomes of creativity always positive? Creativity: Theories-Research-Applications, 5, 177–181. 

  20. Runco M., Beghetto R.A. (2019) Primary and secondary creativity. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 7–10. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.08.011

  21. Runco M.A., Illies J.J., Eisenman R. (2005) Creativity, originality, and appropriateness: What do explicit instructions tell us about their relationships? The Journal of Creative Behavior, 39, 137–148. doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.2005.tb01255.x

  22. Sternberg R. (2018) Yes, Creativity Can Predict Academic Success!Creativity: Theories-Research-Applications, 5, 142–145.

Recieved: 05/14/2020

Accepted: 05/27/2020

Published: 06/22/2020

Keywords: creativity; self-assessment of creativity; imagination; values; diagnostics of creativity; Real Word Divergent Task

Available online since: 22.06.2020

Issue 2, 2020